Mismatch negativity

"Mismatch field" and "MMNM" redirect here.

The mismatch negativity (MMN) or mismatch field (MMF) is a component of the event-related potential (ERP) to an odd stimulus in a sequence of stimuli. It arises from electrical activity in the brain and is studied within the field of cognitive neuroscience and psychology. It can occur in any sensory system, but has most frequently been studied for hearing and for vision. In the case of auditory stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of sounds (sometimes the entire sequence is called an oddball sequence.) For example, a rare deviant (d) sound can be interspersed among a series of frequent standard (s) sounds (e.g., s s s s s s s s s d s s s s s s d s s s d s s s s...). The deviant sound can differ from the standards in one or more perceptual features such as pitch, duration, or loudness. The MMN is usually evoked by either a change in frequency, intensity, duration and real or apparent spatial locus of origin.[1] The MMN can be elicited regardless of whether the subject is paying attention to the sequence.[2] During auditory sequences, a person can be reading or watching a silent subtitled movie, yet still show a clear MMN. In the case of visual stimuli, the MMN occurs after an infrequent change in a repetitive sequence of images.

MMN refers to the mismatch response in electroencephalography (EEG); MMF or MMNM refer to the mismatch response in magnetoencephalography (MEG).

History

The auditory MMN was discovered in 1978 by Risto Näätänen, A. W. K. Gaillard, and S. Mäntysalo at the Institute for Perception, TNO in The Netherlands.[3]

The first report of a visual MMN was in 1990 by Rainer Cammer.[4] For a history of the development of the visual MMN, see Pazo-Alvarez et al. (2003).[5]

Characteristics

The MMN is a response to a deviant within a sequence of otherwise regular stimuli; thus, in an experimental setting, it is produced when stimuli are presented in a many-to-one ratio; for example, in a sequence of sounds s s s s s s s d s s s s d s s s..., the d is the deviant or oddball stimulus, and will elicit an MMN response. The mismatch negativity occurs even if the subject is not consciously paying attention to the stimuli.[3] Processing of sensory stimulus features is essential for humans in determining their responses and actions. If behaviourally relevant aspects of the environment are not correctly represented in the brain, then the organism's behaviour cannot be appropriate. Without these representations our ability to understand spoken language, for example, would be seriously impaired. Cognitive neuroscience has consequently emphasised the importance of understanding brain mechanisms of sensory information processing, that is, the sensory prerequisites of cognition. Most of the data obtained, unfortunately, do not allow the objective measurement of the accuracy of these stimulus representations (see Näätänen, 1992).[6] In addition, recent cognitive neuroscience seems to have succeeded in extracting such a measure, however. This is the mismatch negativity (MMN), a component of the event-related potential (ERP), first reported by Näätänen, Gaillard, and Mäntysalo (1978).[3] An in-depth review of MMN research can be found in Näätänen (1992)[6] while other recent reviews also provide information on the generator mechanisms of MMN (Alho 1995),[7] its magnetic counterpart, MMNm (Näätänen, Ilmoniemi & Alho, 1994),[8] and its clinical applicability (Näätänen & Alho, 1995).[9]

The auditory MMN can occur in response to deviance in pitch, intensity, or duration. The auditory MMN is a fronto-central negative potential with sources in the primary and non-primary auditory cortex and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus. Sources could also include one from the right opercular part of the inferior frontal gyrus. The amplitude and latency of the MMN is related to how different the deviant stimulus is from the standard. Large deviances elicit MMN at earlier latencies. For very large deviances, the MMN can even overlap the N100 (e.g., Campbell et al., 2007).[10]

The visual MMN can occur in response to deviance in such aspects as color, size, or duration. The visual MMN is an occipital negative potential with sources in the primary visual cortex and a typical latency of 150-250 ms after the onset of the deviant stimulus.

Neurolinguistics

As kindred phenomena have been elicited with speech stimuli, under passive conditions that require very little active attention to the sound, a version of MMN has been frequently used in studies of neurolinguistic perception, to test whether or not these participants neurologically distinguish between certain kinds of sounds.[11] In addition to these kinds of studies focusing on phonological processing, some research has implicated the MMN in syntactic processing.[12] Some of these studies have attempted to directly test the automaticity of the MMN, providing converging evidence for the understanding of the MMN as a task-independent and automatic response.[13]

For basic stimulus features

MMN is evoked by an infrequently presented stimulus ("deviant"), differing from the frequently-occurring stimuli ("standards") in one or several physical parameters like duration, intensity, or frequency (Näätänen, 1992).[6] In addition, it is generated by a change in spectrally complex stimuli like phonemes, in synthesised instrumental tones, or in the spectral component of tone timbre. Also the temporal order reversals elicit an MMN when successive sound elements differ either in frequency, intensity, or duration. The MMN is not elicited by stimuli with deviant stimulus parameters when they are presented without the intervening standards. Thus, the MMN has been suggested to reflect change detection when a memory trace representing the constant standard stimulus and the neural code of the stimulus with deviant parameter(s) are discrepant.

Vs. auditory sensory memory

The MMN data can be understood as providing evidence that stimulus features are separately analysed and stored in the vicinity of auditory cortex (for a discussion, please see the theory section below). The close resemblance of the behaviour of the MMN to that of the previously behaviourally observed "echoic" memory system strongly suggests that the MMN provides a non-invasive, objective, task-independently measurable physiological correlate of stimulus-feature representations in auditory sensory memory.

Relationship to attentional processes

The experimental evidence suggests that the auditory sensory memory index MMN provides sensory data for attentional processes, and, in essence, governs certain aspects of attentive information processing. This is evident in the finding that the latency of the MMN determines the timing of behavioural responses to changes in the auditory environment.[14] Furthermore, even individual differences in discrimination ability can be probed with the MMN. The MMN is also a likely component of the chain of brain events causing attention switches to changes in the environment. In the light of these observations, it seems that at present the MMN provides the best available physiological measure of automatic central auditory processing.

In clinical research

The MMN has been documented in a number of studies to disclose neuropathological changes. Presently, the accumulated body of evidence suggests that while the MMN offers unique opportunities to basic research of the information processing of a healthy brain, it might be useful in tapping neurodegenerative changes as well.

MMN, which is elicited irrespective of attention, provides an objective means for evaluating possible auditory discrimination and sensory-memory anomalies in such clinical groups as dyslexics and patients with aphasia, who have a multitude of symptoms including attentional problems. Recent results suggest that a major problem underlying the reading deficit in dyslexia might be an inability of the dyslexics' auditory cortex to adequately model complex sound patterns with fast temporal variation. According to the results of an ongoing study, MMN might also be used in the evaluation of auditory perception deficits in aphasia.

AD patients demonstrate decreased amplitude of MMN, especially with long inter-stimulus intervals; this is thought to reflect reduced span of auditory sensory memory. Parkinsonian patients do demonstrate a similar deficit pattern, whereas alcoholism would appear to enhance the MMN response. This latter, seemingly contradictory, finding could be explained by hyperexcitability of CNS neurones resulting from neuroadaptive changes taking place during a heavy drinking bout.

While the results obtained thus far seem encouraging, several steps need to be taken before the MMN can be used as a clinical tool in patient treatment. A focus of research in the late 1990s aimed to tackle some of the key signal-analysis problems encountered in development of clinical use of MMN and challenges still remain. Nevertheless, as it stands, clinical research employing the MMN has already produced significant knowledge on the CNS functional changes related to cognitive decline in the aforementioned clinical disorders.

A 2010 study found that MMN durations were reduced in a group of schizophrenia patients who later went on to have psychotic episodes, suggesting that MMN durations may predict future psychosis.[15]

Theory

The mainstream "memory trace" interpretation of MMN is that it is elicited in response to violations of simple rules governing the properties of information. It is thought to arise from violation of an automatically formed, short-term neural model or memory trace of physical or abstract environmental regularities (Näätänen & Winkler, 1999; Näätänen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho 2007).[16][17] However, other than MMN, there is no other neurophysiological evidence for the formation of the memory representation of those regularities.

Integral to this memory trace view is that there are: i) a population of sensory afferent neuronal elements that respond to sound, and; ii) a separate population of memory neuronal elements that build a neural model of standard stimulation and respond more vigorously when the incoming stimulation violates that neural model, eliciting an MMN.

An alternative "fresh afferent" interpretation (Näätänen, 1992; Jääskeläinen et al., 2004)[6][18] is that there are no memory neuronal elements, but the sensory afferent neuronal elements that are tuned to properties of the standard stimulation respond less vigorously upon repeated stimulation. Thus when a deviant activates a distinct new population of neuronal elements that is tuned to the different properties of the deviant rather than the standard, these fresh afferents respond more vigorously, eliciting an MMN.

A third view is that the sensory afferents are the memory neurons (Ulanovsky, 2004; Jääskeläinen et al., 2007).[19][20]

See also

References

  1. Näätänen, R.; Paavilainen, P.; Titinen, H.; Jiang, D.; Alho, D. (1993). "Attention and Mismatch Negativity". Psychophysiology. 30 (5): 436–450. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02067.x.
  2. Brattico, Elvira; Tervaniemi, Mari; Näätänen, Risto; Peretz, Isabelle (8 August 2006). "Musical Scale Properties are Automatically Processed in the Human Auditory Cortex" (PDF). Brain Research. Elsevier B.V. 1117 (1): 162–174. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.023. PMID 16963000. Retrieved 20 June 2014.
  3. 1 2 3 Näätänen R, Gaillard AW, Mäntysalo S (July 1978). "Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted". Acta Psychol (Amst). 42 (4): 313–29. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9. PMID 685709.
  4. Cammann, R. (1990). Is there no MMN in the visual modality? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 234-234.
  5. Pazo-Alvarez, P., Cadaveira, F., & Amenedo, E. (2003). MMN in the visual modality: A review. Biological Psychology, 63, 199-236.
  6. 1 2 3 4 Näätänen, Risto (1992). Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum. ISBN 0-8058-0984-8. OCLC 25832590.
  7. Alho K (February 1995). "Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes". Ear Hear. 16 (1): 38–51. doi:10.1097/00003446-199502000-00004. PMID 7774768.
  8. Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Alho K (September 1994). "Magnetoencephalography in studies of human cognitive brain function". Trends Neurosci. 17 (9): 389–95. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(94)90048-5. PMID 7529443.
  9. Näätänen R, Alho K (1995). "Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition". Int. J. Neurosci. 80 (1-4): 317–37. doi:10.3109/00207459508986107. PMID 7775056.
  10. Campbell, T.A.; Winkler, I.; Kujala, T. (2007). "N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: Disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1". Psychophysiology. 44: 530–540. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00529.x.
  11. Phillips, C.; Pellathy, T.; Marantz, A.; Yellin, E.; Wexler, K.; McGinnis, M.; Poeppel, D.; Roberts, T. (2001). "Auditory Cortex Accesses Phonological Category: An MEG Mismatch Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (6): 1038–1055. doi:10.1162/08989290051137567.
  12. Pulvermüller, Friedemann; Yury Shtyrov (2007). "The mismatch negativity as an objective tool for studying higher language functions". Automaticity and Control in Language Processing. Advances in Behavioural Brain Science. Eds. Antje S. Meyer, Linda R. Wheeldon, and Andrea Krott. pp. 217242.
    Specific experimental studies include the following:
    • Hasting, Anna S.; Sonja A. Kotz; Angela D. Friederici (2007). "Setting the Stage for Automatic Syntax Processing: The Mismatch Negativity as an Indicator of Syntactic Priming". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (3): 386400. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.386. PMID 17335388.
    • Hasting, Anna S.; István Winkler; Sonja A. Kotz (2008). "Early differential processing of verbs and nouns in the human brain as indexed by event-related brain potentials". European Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (6): 15611565. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06103.x. PMID 18364028.
    • Pulvermüller, Friedemann; Yury Shtyrov (2003). "Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatch negativity". NeuroImage. 20 (1): 159. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00261-1. PMID 14527578.
    • Pulvermüller, Friedemann; Yury Shtyrov; Anna S. Hasting; Robert P. Carlyon (2008). "Syntax as a reflex: Neurophysiological evidence for the early automaticity of syntactic processing". Brain and Language. 104 (3): 244253. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002. PMID 17624417.
  13. Pulvermüller, Friedemann; Yury Shtyrov; Anna S. Hasting; Robert P. Carlyon (2008). "Syntax as a reflex: Neurophysiological evidence for the early automaticity of syntactic processing". Brain and Language. 104 (3): 244253. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.05.002. PMID 17624417.
  14. Tiitinen, H.; May, P.; Reinikainen, K.; Näätänen, R. "Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory". Nature. 372 (6501): 90–92. doi:10.1038/372090a0.
  15. Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, et al. (May 2011). "Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity". Biol. Psychiatry. 69 (10): 959–66. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.057. PMID 21167475.
  16. Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K (December 2007). "The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review". Clin Neurophysiol. 118 (12): 2544–90. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026. PMID 17931964.
  17. Näätänen R, Winkler I (November 1999). "The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience". Psychol Bull. 125 (6): 826–59. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.826. PMID 10589304.
  18. Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Bonmassar G, et al. (April 2004). "Human posterior auditory cortex gates novel sounds to consciousness". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101 (17): 6809–14. doi:10.1073/pnas.0303760101. PMC 404127Freely accessible. PMID 15096618.
  19. Ulanovsky, N. (2004). "Neuronal adaptation in cat auditory cortex" (pdf).
  20. Jääskeläinen IP, Ahveninen J, Belliveau JW, Raij T, Sams M (December 2007). "Short-term plasticity in auditory cognition". Trends Neurosci. 30 (12): 653–61. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.09.003. PMID 17981345.

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/8/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.